Uncovering temporal structure in hippocampal output patterns

  1. Kourosh Maboudi
  2. Etienne Ackermann
  3. Laurel Watkins de Jong
  4. Brad Pfeiffer
  5. David Foster
  6. Kamran Diba  Is a corresponding author
  7. Caleb Kemere  Is a corresponding author
  1. University of Michigan, United States
  2. Rice University, United States
  3. University of Texas Southwestern, United States
  4. University of California, Berkeley, United States

Abstract

Place cell activity of hippocampal pyramidal cells has been described as the cognitive substrate of spatial memory. Replay is observed during hippocampal sharp-wave-ripple-associated population burst events (PBEs) and is critical for consolidation and recall-guided behaviors. PBE activity has historically been analyzed as a phenomenon subordinate to the place code. Here, we use hidden Markov models to study PBEs observed in rats during exploration of both linear mazes and open fields. We demonstrate that estimated models are consistent with a spatial map of the environment, and can even decode animals' positions during behavior. Moreover, we demonstrate the model can be used to identify hippocampal replay without recourse to the place code, using only PBE model congruence. These results suggest that downstream regions may rely on PBEs to provide a substrate for memory. Additionally, by forming models independent of animal behavior, we lay the groundwork for studies of non-spatial memory.

Data availability

We analyzed data from neural recording experiments. Data for Figures 1-5 has been previously reported in [1]. These data and also data for Figure 8 are available from Kamran Diba on request. Data for Figure 6 was previously reported in [2] and is available from Brad Pfeiffer and David Foster on request. Data for Figure 7 was previously reported in [3] is available from the CRCNS.org archive ('hc-6'). [1] Diba and Buzsaki, Nature Neuroscience, 2007 [2] Pfeiffer and Foster, Science, 2015 [3] Karlsson and Frank, Nature Neuroscience, 2009.All analysis code and sample recording epochs for Figures 1-7 are available on https://github.com/kemerelab/UncoveringTemporalStructureHippocampus. These make use of our broader open-source Python analysis software https://github.com/nelpy.

The following previously published data sets were used

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Kourosh Maboudi

    Department of Anesthesiology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, United States
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  2. Etienne Ackermann

    Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, Rice University, Houston, United States
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  3. Laurel Watkins de Jong

    Department of Anesthesiology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, United States
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  4. Brad Pfeiffer

    Department of Neuroscience, University of Texas Southwestern, Dallas, United States
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  5. David Foster

    Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, United States
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  6. Kamran Diba

    Department of Anesthesiology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, United States
    For correspondence
    kdiba@umich.edu
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-5128-4478
  7. Caleb Kemere

    Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, Rice University, Houston, United States
    For correspondence
    caleb.kemere@rice.edu
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0003-2054-0234

Funding

National Science Foundation (IOS-1550994)

  • Etienne Ackermann
  • Caleb Kemere

Human Frontier Science Program (RGY0088)

  • Etienne Ackermann
  • Caleb Kemere

Ken Kennedy Institute (ERIT)

  • Caleb Kemere

National Institute of Mental Health (R01MH109170)

  • Kourosh Maboudi
  • Kamran Diba

National Institute of Mental Health (R01MH085823)

  • Brad Pfeiffer
  • David Foster

Alfred P. Sloan Foundation

  • Brad Pfeiffer
  • David Foster

Brain and Behavior Research Foundation (NARSAD Young Investigator Grant)

  • Brad Pfeiffer
  • David Foster

McKnight Endowment Fund for Neuroscience

  • David Foster

National Science Foundation (CBET-1351692)

  • Etienne Ackermann

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Reviewing Editor

  1. Laura Colgin, The University of Texas at Austin, Center for Learning and Memory, United States

Ethics

Animal experimentation: As reported previously, all procedures were approved by the Johns Hopkins University, Rutgers University, and University of California, San Francisco Animal Care and Use Committees and followed US National Institutes of Health animal use guidelines (protocol 90-042).

Version history

  1. Received: December 20, 2017
  2. Accepted: May 14, 2018
  3. Accepted Manuscript published: June 5, 2018 (version 1)
  4. Version of Record published: June 21, 2018 (version 2)

Copyright

© 2018, Maboudi et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License permitting unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 5,620
    views
  • 858
    downloads
  • 51
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Kourosh Maboudi
  2. Etienne Ackermann
  3. Laurel Watkins de Jong
  4. Brad Pfeiffer
  5. David Foster
  6. Kamran Diba
  7. Caleb Kemere
(2018)
Uncovering temporal structure in hippocampal output patterns
eLife 7:e34467.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.34467

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.34467

Further reading

    1. Biochemistry and Chemical Biology
    2. Neuroscience
    Maximilian Nagel, Marco Niestroj ... Marc Spehr
    Research Article

    In most mammals, conspecific chemosensory communication relies on semiochemical release within complex bodily secretions and subsequent stimulus detection by the vomeronasal organ (VNO). Urine, a rich source of ethologically relevant chemosignals, conveys detailed information about sex, social hierarchy, health, and reproductive state, which becomes accessible to a conspecific via vomeronasal sampling. So far, however, numerous aspects of social chemosignaling along the vomeronasal pathway remain unclear. Moreover, since virtually all research on vomeronasal physiology is based on secretions derived from inbred laboratory mice, it remains uncertain whether such stimuli provide a true representation of potentially more relevant cues found in the wild. Here, we combine a robust low-noise VNO activity assay with comparative molecular profiling of sex- and strain-specific mouse urine samples from two inbred laboratory strains as well as from wild mice. With comprehensive molecular portraits of these secretions, VNO activity analysis now enables us to (i) assess whether and, if so, how much sex/strain-selective ‘raw’ chemical information in urine is accessible via vomeronasal sampling; (ii) identify which chemicals exhibit sufficient discriminatory power to signal an animal’s sex, strain, or both; (iii) determine the extent to which wild mouse secretions are unique; and (iv) analyze whether vomeronasal response profiles differ between strains. We report both sex- and, in particular, strain-selective VNO representations of chemical information. Within the urinary ‘secretome’, both volatile compounds and proteins exhibit sufficient discriminative power to provide sex- and strain-specific molecular fingerprints. While total protein amount is substantially enriched in male urine, females secrete a larger variety at overall comparatively low concentrations. Surprisingly, the molecular spectrum of wild mouse urine does not dramatically exceed that of inbred strains. Finally, vomeronasal response profiles differ between C57BL/6 and BALB/c animals, with particularly disparate representations of female semiochemicals.

    1. Neuroscience
    Kenta Abe, Yuki Kambe ... Tatsuo Sato
    Research Article

    Midbrain dopamine neurons impact neural processing in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) through mesocortical projections. However, the signals conveyed by dopamine projections to the PFC remain unclear, particularly at the single-axon level. Here, we investigated dopaminergic axonal activity in the medial PFC (mPFC) during reward and aversive processing. By optimizing microprism-mediated two-photon calcium imaging of dopamine axon terminals, we found diverse activity in dopamine axons responsive to both reward and aversive stimuli. Some axons exhibited a preference for reward, while others favored aversive stimuli, and there was a strong bias for the latter at the population level. Long-term longitudinal imaging revealed that the preference was maintained in reward- and aversive-preferring axons throughout classical conditioning in which rewarding and aversive stimuli were paired with preceding auditory cues. However, as mice learned to discriminate reward or aversive cues, a cue activity preference gradually developed only in aversive-preferring axons. We inferred the trial-by-trial cue discrimination based on machine learning using anticipatory licking or facial expressions, and found that successful discrimination was accompanied by sharper selectivity for the aversive cue in aversive-preferring axons. Our findings indicate that a group of mesocortical dopamine axons encodes aversive-related signals, which are modulated by both classical conditioning across days and trial-by-trial discrimination within a day.