Introduction

Animals are constantly sensing environmental stimuli and changing their behavior or physiology based on their internal state 17. Hungry animals are desperate for food. Immediately after seeing, smelling, or chewing food, even without absorbing nutrients, a burst of physiological changes is suddenly initiated in the body. These responses are known in mammals as the cephalic phase response (CPR). For example, a flood of saliva and gastrointestinal secretions prepares hungry animals to digest food 812. Starvation results in lower body temperatures, and chewing food triggers a rapid increase in heat production, demonstrating CPR in thermogenesis 1315. Hungry animals are strongly attracted to food. However, the underlying mechanisms of how the sensation of food without nutrients triggers the behavioral response remain unclear.

To address this question, we used a relatively simple and versatile model organism, Drosophila melanogaster. Flies exhibit robust temperature preference behavior 16,17. Due to the low mass of small ectotherms, their body temperatures are close to the ambient temperature 18,19. Hunger stress forces flies to change their behavior and physiological response 20,21. The hungry flies prefer a lower temperature 22, indicating lower body temperatures and lower metabolic rates 2224. Notably, this phenomenon of hunger-driven lower body temperature is similar to mammals, as the body temperature of starved animals is lower than that of fed animals 57. Furthermore, flies exhibit robust feeding behavior 25,26, and molecular and neural mechanisms of taste are well documented 2,2732. Therefore, we asked whether the taste cue triggers a robust behavioral recovery of temperature preference in starving flies.

Here we show in hungry flies that taste without nutrients induces a switch from a low to a high temperature preference. While taste leads to a warmer temperature preference, nutrient intake causes the flies to prefer an even warmer temperature. This nutrient-induced warm preference results in a complete recovery from starvation. Thus, taste-evoked warm preference is different from nutrient-induced warm preference and potentially similar physiology as CPR. Therefore, when animals emerge from starvation, they use a two-step approach to recovery: taste-evoked and nutrient-induced warm preference. While a rapid component is elicited by food taste alone, a slower component requires nutrient intake.

Animals are motivated to eat based on their internal state, such as time of day or degree of hunger. The circadian clock drives daily feeding rhythms 3335 and anticipates meal timing. Daily feeding timing influences energy homeostasis and metabolism 36,37. Hunger and satiety dramatically affect animal behavior and physiology 17 and control feeding. We found that clock genes and hunger signals are strongly required for taste-evoked warm preference. The data suggest that taste-evoked response is an indispensable physiological response that represents internal state. Taken together, our data shed new light on the role of the taste-evoked response and highlight a crucial aspect of our understanding of feeding state and energy homeostasis.

Results

Food detection triggers a warm preference

Flies exhibit robust temperature preference behavior 16,17. To examine Drosophila temperature preference behavior, flies are released into a chamber with a temperature gradient of 16-34°C 38 and subsequently accumulate at their preferred temperature (Tp) within 30 min. The white1118 (w1118) flies were fed fly food containing carbohydrate, protein and fat sources (Fig. 1A) and preferred 25.2±0.2 OC (Fig. 1B: fed, white bar). When w1118 flies were starved overnight with water only, they preferred 21.7±0.3 OC (Fig. 1A, 1B: overnight starvation (STV), gray bar). Thus, starvation leads to a lower Tp. As we have previously reported, starvation strongly influences temperature preference 22.

Hungry flies switch from cold to warm preference upon food detection.

(A) A schematic diagram of the experimental conditions. Flies were reared on fly food. Emerging flies were kept on fly food for one or two days (orange) and then starved with water-absorbing paper for one or two nights (gray). Starved flies were refed with either fly food (orange), 2 mL sucralose (blue), or glucose solution (green) absorbed paper and then examined for temperature preference (Tp) behavioral assays. Details are provided in the experimental procedures.

(B-D) Comparison of Tp of white1118 (w1118) control flies between fed (white bar), starved (STV; gray bar), and refed (orange, blue, or green bar) states. Starved flies were refed with fly food (orange bar) for 5, 10, 30, or 60 min (1 hr) (B), 2.8 mM sucralose solution (blue bar) for 10 min or 1 hr (C), or 2.8 mM (equivalent to 5%) glucose solution (green bar) for 5 min, 10 min, or 1 hr (D). Behavioral experiments were performed at the specific time points, Zeitgeber time (ZT) 4-7. ZT0 and ZT12 are light on and light off, respectively. Dots on each bar indicate individual Tp in assays. Numbers in italics indicate the number of trials. The Shapiro-Wilk test was performed to test for normality. One-way ANOVA was performed to compare Tp between each refeeding condition. Tukey’s post hoc test was used to compare differences between experimental and starved conditions (green stars). Data are presented as mean Tp with SEM. *p < 0.05. **p < 0.01. ***p < 0.001. ****p < 0.0001. NS indicates no significance.

(E) Comparison of Tp among the following conditions in w1118 (control) flies: fed (white bar), starved overnight (O/N) (water or STV; gray bar), refed with fly food (fly food; orange bar), sucralose (blue bars), or glucose (green bars). The same data are used in Fig. 1B-D. The Shapiro-Wilk test was performed to test for normality. One-way ANOVA was performed to compare Tp between each refeeding condition. Red (or green) stars indicate Tukey’s post hoc test comparing differences between experimental and fed (or starved) conditions, respectively. *p < 0.05. **p < 0.01. ***p < 0.001. ****p < 0.0001. NS indicates not significant.

(F) Feeding assay: The number of touches to water (gray), 2.8 mM (equivalent to 5%) glucose (green), or 2.8 mM sucralose in each solution (blue) was examined using w1118 flies starved for 1 day. A cumulative number of touches to water or sugar solution for 0-30 min was plotted. Two-way ANOVA was used for multiple comparisons. Blue and green stars show Fisher’s LSD post hoc test comparing sucralose (blue stars) or glucose (green stars) solution feeding to water drinking. All data shown are means with SEM. *p < 0.05. **p < 0.01. ***p < 0.001. ****p < 0.0001.

To examine how starved flies recover from lower Tp, they were offered fly food (Fig. 1A) and their Tp was tested. After 10 min, 30 min or 1 hr of fly food refeeding, starved flies preferred a temperature similar to that of fed flies (Fig. 1B: statistics shown as green stars, Table S1), suggesting a full recovery from starvation. On the other hand, refeeding after 5 min resulted in a warmer temperature than the starved flies. Nevertheless, Tp did not reach that of the fed flies (Fig. 1B). Therefore, 5 min fly food refeeding caused a partial recovery from the starved state (Fig. 1E, statistics shown as red stars, Table S1). Thus, our data suggest that food intake triggers a warm preference in starved flies.

Sucralose refeeding promotes a warm preference

Given that only 5 min refeeding caused hungry flies to prefer a warmer temperature (Fig. 1B), we hypothesized that food-sensing cues might be important for the warm preference. Sucralose is an artificial sweetener that activates sweet taste neurons in Drosophila 39 and modulates taste behaviors such as the proboscis extension reflex 4043. Importantly, sucralose is a non-metabolizable sugar and has no calories. Therefore, to investigate how food-sensing cues are involved in warm preference, we examined how sucralose refeeding changes the temperature preference of starved flies. After starved flies were refed sucralose for 10 min or 1 hr (Fig. 1A), they preferred a warmer temperature; however, Tp was halfway between Tps of fed and starved flies (Fig. 1C: blue, Table S1). Thus, sucralose ingestion induces a warm preference but shows a partial recovery of Tp from the starved state.

While refeeding the flies with food resulted in a full recovery of Tp from the starved state, refeeding them with sucralose resulted in only a partial recovery of Tp. Therefore, starved flies may use both taste cues and nutrients to fully recover Tp from the starved state. To evaluate this possibility, we used glucose, which contains both sweetness (gustatory cues) and nutrients (i.e., metabolizable sugar) (Fig. 1A), and tested glucose refeeding for 5 min, 10 min, and 1 hr. We found that 10 min or 1 hr glucose refeeding resulted in full recovery of Tp from the starved state (Fig. 1D and E: statistics shown in red NS, Table S1) and was significantly different from starved flies (Fig. 1D and E: statistics shown in green stars, Table S1). Thus, our data showed that sucralose refeeding induced partial recovery and glucose refeeding induced full recovery from the starved state.

However, it is still possible that the starved flies consumed glucose more or faster than sucralose during the first 10 min, which could result in a different warming preference. To rule out this possibility, we examined how often starved flies touched glucose, sucralose, or water during the 30 min using the Fly Liquid-food Interaction Counter (FLIC) system 44. The FLIC system assays allow us to monitor how much interaction between the fly and liquid food reflects feeding/drinking episodes. We found that starved flies touched glucose and sucralose food at similar frequencies and more frequently than water during the 30 min test period (Fig. 1F, Table S1). The data suggest that flies are likely to consume glucose and sucralose at similar rates and amounts. Therefore, we concluded that the differential effect of sucralose and glucose refeeding on temperature preference was not due to differences in feeding rate or amount. Instead, the gustatory cues likely induce partial Tp recovery, and the gustatory cue plus nutrients induce full Tp recovery.

Activation of sweet taste neurons leads to warm preference

We first focused on how taste elicits a warm preference. Sweet gustatory receptors (Grs) detect sweet taste. We used sweet Gr mutants and asked whether sweet Grs are involved in taste-evoked warm preference. Two different sweet Gr mutant strains, Gr5a-/-; Gr64a-/- and Gr5a-/-;;Gr61a-/-, Gr64a-f-/-, have reduced sugar sensitivity compared to the control 41,42,45,46. We found that sweet Gr mutant flies exhibited a normal starvation response in which the Tp of starved flies was lower than that of fed flies (Fig. 2A and B, white and gray bars, Table S1). However, starved sweet Gr mutant flies did not increase Tp after 10 min sucralose refeeding (Fig. 2A and B, blue bars, Table S1). These data suggest that sweet Grs are involved in taste-evoked warm preference.

Gustatory neurons are essential for taste-evoked warm preference.

(A-E) Comparison of Tp of flies between fed (white bar), starved (STV; gray bar), and refed (blue bar) states. Starved flies were refed with sucralose (blue bars) for 10 min. (F) Schematic of optogenetic activation assay.

(G-J) Comparison of Tp of flies between fed (white bar), starved (STV; gray bar), and starved with all-trans-retinal (ATR; yellow bars), which is the chromophore required for CsChrimson activation. Gustatory neurons in starved flies were excited by red light pulses (flashing on and off at 10 Hz) for 10 min. Behavioral experiments were conducted at ZT4-7. Dots on each bar indicate individual Tp in assays. Numbers in italics indicate the number of trials. Shapiro-Wilk test was performed for normality test. One-way ANOVA was used for statistical analysis. Green stars indicate Tukey’s post hoc test compared between each experiment and the starved condition. Pooled data are shown in Fig. S3. All data shown are means with SEM. *p < 0.05. **p < 0.01. ***p < 0.001. ****p < 0.0001. NS indicates not significant.

Sweet Grs are expressed in the sweet Gr-expressing neurons (GRNs) located in the proboscis and forelegs 46,47. To determine whether sweet GRNs are involved in taste-evoked warm preference, we silenced all sweet GRNs. We expressed the inward rectifying K+ channel Kir2.1 (uas-Kir) 48 with Gr64f-Gal4, which is expressed in all sweet GRNs in the proboscis and forelegs 41,46,47. Knockdown of all sweet GRNs showed a normal starvation response (Fig. 2C, white and gray bars). However, flies silencing all sweet GRNs failed to show a warm preference after 10 min sucralose refeeding (Fig. 2C, blue bar, Table S1). This phenotype was similar to the data obtained with the sweet Gr mutant strains (Fig. 2A and B). On the other hand, control flies (Gr64f-Gal4/+ and uas-Kir/+) showed a normal starvation response and a taste-evoked warm preference (Fig. 2D and E, gray and blue bars, respectively, Table S1). Thus, our data indicate that sweet GRNs are required for taste-evoked warm preference.

To further investigate whether activation of sweet GRNs induces a warm preference, we used the optogenetic approach, a red light sensitive channelrhodopsin, CsChrimson 49,50. Starved flies were given water containing 0.8 mM all-trans-retinal (ATR), the chromophore required for CsChrimson activation. These flies were not fed sucralose; instead, gustatory neurons in starved flies were excited by red light pulses (flashing on and off at 10 Hz) for 10 min (Fig. 2F). In this case, although the flies were not refed, the gustatory neurons were artificially excited by CsChrimson activation so that we could evaluate the effect of excitation of sweet GRNs on taste-evoked warm preference.

CsChrimson was expressed in sweet GRNs in the proboscis and legs (all sweet GRNs) using Gr64f-Gal4. These flies showed a normal starvation response (Fig. 2G-J, white and gray bars, Table S1). Excitation of all sweet GRNs by red light pulses elicited a warm preference, and Tp was intermediate between fed and starved flies, suggesting partial recovery (Fig. 2G, yellow bar, Table S1). However, neither excitation of Gr5a-(Fig. 2H) nor Gr64a-expressing neurons (Fig. 2I) induced a warm preference (yellow bars, Table S1). While Gr64a-Gal4 is expressed only in legs, Gr5a-Gal4 is expressed in proboscis and legs, but does not cover all sweet GRNs like Gr64f-Gal4 46,47. Notably, control flies (UAS-CsChrimson/+) did not show a warm preference to red light pulses with ATR application (Fig. 2J). Taken together, our data suggest that excitation of all sweet GRNs results in a warm preference.

The temperature-sensing neurons are involved in taste-evoked warm preference

The warm-sensing neurons, anterior cells (ACs) and the cold-sensing R11F02-Gal4-expressing neurons control temperature preference behavior 38,51,52. Small ectotherms such as Drosophila set their Tp to avoid noxious temperatures using temperature information from cold- and warm-sensing neurons 16,17,38. We have previously shown that starved flies choose a lower Tp, the so-called hunger-driven lower Tp 22. ACs control hunger-driven lower Tp but cold-sensing R11F02-Gal4-expressing neurons do not 22. ACs express transient receptor potential A1 (TrpA1), which responds to a warm temperature >25 OC 38,53. The set point of ACs in fed flies, which is ∼25 OC, is lowered in starved flies. Therefore, the lower set point of ACs corresponds to the lower Tp in starved flies.

First, we asked whether ACs are involved in taste-evoked warm preference. Because the ACs are important for the hunger-driven lower Tp 22, the AC-silenced flies did not show a significant difference in Tp between fed and starved conditions for only one overnight starvation 22. Therefore, we first extended the starvation time to two nights so that the AC-silenced flies showed a significant difference in Tp between fed and starved conditions (Fig. 3A, white and gray bars, Table S1). Importantly, more prolonged starvation does not affect the ability of w1118 flies to recover (Fig. S1).

Both warm- and cold-temperature-sensing neurons are involved with taste-evoked warm preference.

(A-E) Comparison of Tp of flies between fed (white bar), starved (STV; gray bar), and refed (blue bar) conditions. Starved flies were refed with sucralose (blue bars) for 10 min. The Shapiro-Wilk test was performed to test for normality. One-way ANOVA or Kruskal-Wallis test was used for statistical analysis. Green stars indicate Tukey’s post hoc test or Dunn’s test compared between each experiment and the starved condition. Pooled data are shown in Fig. S3. (F and G) Comparison of Tp between starved (STV; gray bar) and all-trans-retinal (ATR; yellow bars) starved flies. Warm neurons (F) or cold neurons (G) in starved flies expressed CsChrimson, which was excited by red light pulses for 10 min. The Shapiro-Wilk test was performed to test for normality. Student’s t-test or Kolmogorov-Smirnov test was used for statistical analysis. (G) tubGal80ts; R11F02-Gal4>uas-CsChrimson flies were reared at 18°C, and emerged adults were collected and stored at 29°C. See Materials and Methods for details. These behavioral experiments were conducted on ZT4-7. All data shown are means with SEM. *p < 0.05. **p < 0.01. ***p < 0.001. ****p < 0.0001. NS indicates no significance. The same data from UAS-Kir/+ in Fig. 2E are used in Fig. 3E.

To examine whether ACs regulate taste-evoked warm preference, we refed sucralose to AC-silenced flies for 10 min. We found that the Tp of the refed flies was still similar to that of the starved flies (Figure 3A, blue bar, Table S1), indicating that sucralose refeeding could not restore Tp and that ACs are involved in taste-evoked warm preference. Significantly, even when the AC-silenced flies were starved for two nights, they were able to recover Tp to the normal food for 10 min and to glucose for 1 hr (Fig. S3D), suggesting that the starved AC-silenced flies were still capable of recovery.

Other temperature-sensing neurons involved in temperature preference behavior are cold-sensing R11F02-Gal4-expressing neurons 51,52. To determine whether R11F02-Gal4-expressing neurons are involved in taste-evoked warm preference, we silenced R11F02-Gal4-expressing neurons using uas-Kir. We found that the flies showed a significant difference in Tp between fed and starved conditions, but the flies did not show a warm preference upon sucralose refeeding (Fig. 3B, gray and blue bars, Table S1). As controls, TrpA1SH-Gal4/+, R11F02-Gal4/+ and uas-Kir/+ flies showed normal starvation response and taste-evoked warm preference (Fig. 3C-E).

To further ensure the results, we used optogenetics to artificially excite warm and cold neurons with TrpA1SH-Gal4 and R11F02-Gal4, respectively, by red light pulses for 10 min. We compared the Tp of starved flies with and without ATR under red light. Tp of starved flies with ATR was significantly increased (Fig. 3F and G, yellow bars, Table S1) compared to those without ATR (Fig. 3F and G, gray bars, Table S1). Therefore, these data indicate that ACs and R11F02-Gal4-expressing neurons are required for taste-evoked warm preference.

Internal state influences taste-evoked warm preference in hungry flies

Internal state strongly influences feeding motivation. However, how internal state influences starved animals to exhibit a food response remains unclear. Hunger represents the food-deficient state in the body, which induces the release of hunger signals such as neuropeptide Y (NPY). NPY promotes foraging and feeding behavior in mammals and flies 54. While intracerebroventricular injection of NPY induces cephalic phase response (CPR) 55, injection of NPY antagonists suppresses CPR in dogs, suggesting that NPY is a regulator of CPR in mammals 56. Therefore, we first focused on neuropeptide F (NPF) and small neuropeptide F (sNPF), which are the Drosophila homologue and orthologue of mammalian NPY, respectively 54, and asked whether they are involved in taste-evoked warm preference. In NPF mutant (NPF-/-) or sNPF hypomorph (sNPF hypo) mutant, we found that the Tp of fed and starved flies were significantly different, showing a normal starved response (Fig. 4A and B: white vs. gray bars, Table S1). However, they failed to show a taste-evoked warm preference after 10 min sucralose refeeding (Fig. 4A and B: blue bars, Table S1). Thus, NPF and sNPF are required for taste-evoked warm preference after sucralose feeding.

Hunger signals are involved in taste-evoked warm preference.

Comparison of Tp of flies between fed (white bar), starved (STV; gray bar), and refed states (orange, blue, green, or dark green). Starved flies were refed with fly food (orange bar), sucralose (blue bar), or glucose (green bar) for 10 min. These behavioral experiments were conducted on ZT4-7. Dots on each bar indicate individual Tp in assays. Numbers in italics indicate the number of trials. Shapiro-Wilk test was performed for normality test. One-way ANOVA was used for statistical analysis. Green stars indicate Tukey’s post hoc test compared between each experiment and the starved condition. Pooled data are shown in Fig. S3. All data shown are means with SEM. *p < 0.05. **p < 0.01. ***p < 0.001. ****p < 0.0001. NS indicates not significant.

Next, we asked whether NPF and sNPF are involved in the nutrient-induced warm preference during glucose refeeding. We found that starved NPF-/- mutants significantly increased Tp after 1 hr glucose refeeding (Fig. 4A, green and dark green bars, Table S1). We also found that starved sNPF hypo mutants significantly increased Tp after 10 min and 1 hr of glucose refeeding (Fig. 4B, green and dark green bars, Table S1). In addition, both NPF-/- and sNPF hypo mutants increased Tp after 10 min fly food refeeding (Fig. 4A and B, orange bars, Table S1). These data suggest that NPF and sNPF are unlikely to be critical for nutrient-induced warm preference.

Factors involving hunger regulate taste-evoked warm preference

Based on the above results, we hypothesized that hunger signals might be involved in taste-evoked warm preference. To test this hypothesis, we focused on several factors involved in the hunger state. The major hunger signals 20, diuretic hormone 44 (DH44), and adipokinetic hormone (AKH) are the mammalian corticotropin-releasing hormone homologue 57 and the functional glucagon homologue respectively 58,59. We found that DH44- or AKH-neuron-silenced flies failed to show a taste-evoked warm preference after sucralose refeeding. However, they were able to increase Tp after 10 min glucose or fly food refeeding (Fig. 4D and F, green and orange bars, Table S1), suggesting a normal nutrient-induced warm preference. The control flies (Dh44-Gal4/+, Akh-Gal4/+, and UAS-Kir/+) showed normal warm preference to sucralose (Fig. 4C, E and J, blue bars, Table S1), glucose (Fig. 4C, E and J, green bars, Table S1), and normal fly food refeeding (Fig. 4C, E and J, orange bars, Table S1). The data suggest that DH44 or AKH neurons are required for taste-evoked warm preference, but not for nutrient-induced warm preference.

Insulin-like peptide 6 (Ilp6) is a homologue of mammalian insulin-like growth factor 1 (IGF1), and ilp6 mRNA expression increases in starved flies 6062. Because Ilp6 is important for the hunger-driven lower Tp 22, the ilp6 mutants did not show a significant difference in Tp between fed and starved conditions for only one overnight starvation 22. Therefore, we first extended the starvation time to three nights. We found that ilp6 loss-of-function (ilp6 LOF) mutant failed to show a taste-evoked warm preference after sucralose refeeding (Fig. 4G, blue bars, Table S1), but showed a nutrient-induced warm preference after glucose refeeding (Fig. 4G, green bars, Table S1). These data suggest that Ilp6 is required for taste-evoked warm preference but not for nutrient-induced warm preference after glucose or fly food refeeding.

Unpaired3 (Upd3) is a Drosophila cytokine that is upregulated under nutritional stress 63. We found that upd3 mutants failed to show a taste-evoked warm preference after sucralose refeeding (Fig. 4H, blue bars, Table S1), but showed a nutrient-induced warm preference after glucose refeeding (Fig. 4G and H, green bars, Table S1). These data suggest that Upd3 is required for taste-evoked warm preference, but not for nutrient-induced warm preference after glucose refeeding. Notably, upd3 mutants failed to show nutrient-induced warm preference upon normal food feeding (Fig. 4H, orange bars, Table S1).

We also examined the role of the satiety factor Unpaired2 (Upd2), a functional leptin homologue in flies, in taste-evoked warm preference 64. upd2 mutants failed to show a warm preference after refeeding of sucralose, glucose or fly food (Fig. 4I, blue, green and orange bars, Table S1). Thus, our data suggest that factors involved in the hunger state are required for taste-evoked warm preference.

Flies show a taste-evoked warm preference at all times of the day

Animals anticipate feeding schedules at a time of day that is tightly controlled by the circadian clock 37. Flies show a rhythmic feeding pattern: one peak in the morning 44 or two peaks in the morning and evening 65. Because food cues induce a warm preference, we wondered whether feeding rhythm and taste-evoked warm preference are coordinated. If so, they should show a parallel phenotype.

Since flies exhibit one of the circadian outputs, the temperature preference rhythm (TPR) 66, Tp gradually increases during the day and peaks in the evening. First, we tested starvation responses at Zeitgeber time (ZT) 1-3, 4-6, 7-9, and 10-12 in light and dark (LD) conditions. We found that both w1118 and yellow1 white1 (y1w1) flies exhibited higher Tp in the fed state and lower Tp in the starved state at all times during the day (Fig. 5A and B: black vs. gray line; Table S1). Next, we refed sucralose to starved flies at all time points tested and examined taste-evoked warm preference. While starved y1w1 flies showed a taste-evoked warm preference at all time points (Fig. 5B1, gray vs. blue line, Table S1), starved w1118 flies showed a significant taste-evoked warm preference at ZT 4-6 and 10-12 (Fig. 5A1, gray vs. blue line, Table S1).

Clock genes are involved in taste-evoked warm preference.

(A-D) Comparison of Tp of flies between fed (white circles), starved (STV; gray triangles), and refed (blue, green, or dark green squares) states. Flies were starved for 24 hrs. Starved flies were refed with sucralose (blue squares, A1-D1) or glucose for 10 min (green squares, A2-D2) or 1 hr (dark green squares, A3). After the 10 min or 1 hr refeeding, the temperature preference behavior assays were performed immediately at ZT1-3, 4-6, 7-9, and 10-12 in LD. The Shapiro-Wilk test was performed to test for normality. One-way ANOVA or Kruskal-Wallis test was used for statistical analysis. Stars indicate Tukey’s post hoc test or Dunn’s test compared between each experiment and starved condition at the same time point. All data shown are means with SEM. *p<0.05. **p<0.01. ***p<0.001. ****p < 0.0001.

(E) Comparison of Tp during daytime in each feeding state. One-way ANOVA or Kruskal-Wallis test was used for statistical analysis between ZT1-3 and ZT7-9 or ZT10-12. R and AR indicate rhythmic and arrhythmic, respectively, during the daytime.

Because starved w1118 flies showed an advanced phase shift of TPR with a peak at ZT 7-9 (Fig. 5A, gray), it is likely that the highest Tp simply masks the taste-evoked warm preference at ZT 7-9. We also focused on nutrient- (carbohydrate-) induced warm preference. Starved w1118 and y1w1 flies successfully increased Tp after 10 min glucose refeeding (Fig. 5A2 and B2, green line, Table S1). Glucose refeeding for 1 hr resulted in Tp similar to that of fed w1118 flies (Fig. 5A3, green line). Because the feeding rhythm peaks in the morning or morning/evening 44,65, our data suggest that the feeding rhythm and taste-evoked warm preference do not occur in parallel.

Circadian clock genes are required for taste-evoked warm preference, but not nutrient-induced warm preference

We asked whether the circadian clock is involved in taste-evoked warm preference. We used clock gene null mutants, period01 (per01) and timeless01 (tim01). Although they showed significant starvation responses (Fig. 5C and D, black and gray lines, Table S1), neither starved per01 nor tim01 mutants could exhibit taste-evoked warm preference upon sucralose refeeding (Fig. 5C1 and D1, blue lines, Table S1). Nevertheless, they fully recovered upon glucose refeeding in LD at any time of day (Fig. 5C2 and D2, green lines, Table S1). Therefore, our data suggest that clock genes are required for taste-evoked warm preference, but not for nutrient-induced warm preference.

However, starved per01 and tim01 mutants may eat sucralose less frequently than glucose, which could result in a failure to show a taste-evoked warm preference. Therefore, we examined how often starved per01 and tim01 mutants touched glucose, sucralose, or water during the 30 min using FLIC assays 44 (Fig. S2A-C). Interestingly, starved per01 and tim01 mutants touched water significantly more often than glucose or sucralose (Fig. S2B and C, Table S1). Although starved tim01 flies touched glucose slightly more than sucralose for only 10 min, this phenotype is not consistent with per01 and w1118 flies. Thus, we conclude that in per01 and w1118 flies, the differential response between taste-evoked and nutrient-induced warm preferences is not due to feeding rate or amount.

The sweet GRNs and temperature-sensing neurons contribute both taste-evoked and nutrient (carbohydrate)-induced warm preference

Our data suggest that taste-evoked and nutrient- (carbohydrate-) induced warm preferences are controlled separately. We examined how these differences are coordinated. We show that genes involved in internal state tend to control taste-evoked warm preferences. Since gustatory or temperature-sensitive neurons are required for taste-evoked warm preference (Figs. 2 and 3), we asked whether they contribute to nutrient-induced warm preference.

To this end, we used all-sweet-GRNs-silenced flies (Gr64f-Gal4>uas-Kir), two Grs mutant strains (Gr5a-/-; Gr64a-/- and Gr5a-/-;;Gr61a-/-, Gr64a-f-/-), and the warm- or cold-neuron-silenced flies (TrpA1SH-Gal4 or R11F02-Gal4>uas-Kir). We found that all these starved flies did not increase Tp after 10 min glucose intake (Fig. S3A-E, green bars, Table S1), but increased Tp after 10 min refeeding with fly food containing carbohydrate, fat, and protein (Fig. S3A-E, orange bars, Table S1). As an exception, AC-silenced flies increased Tp after 1 hr glucose intake (Fig. S3D, green bars, Table S1). All control flies showed normal responses to both 10 min glucose refeeding and fly food intake (Fig. S3F-I, green bars, Table S1). The data suggest that gustatory neurons and temperature-sensing neurons are required for warm preference in carbohydrate refeeding, but not in other foods such as fat or protein (see Discussion). Because flies have sensory neurons that detect fatty acids 6769 or amino acids 7073, these neurons may drive the response to fly food intake. This is likely why the sweet- or temperature-insensitive flies can still recover after eating fly food (Fig S3A-E, orange bars).

Discussion

When animals are hungry, sensory detection of food (sight, smell, or chewing) initiates digestion even before the food enters the stomach. The food-evoked responses are also observed in thermogenesis, heart rate, and respiratory rate in mammals 13,14,74. These responses are referred to as the cephalic phase response (CPR) and contribute to the physiological regulation of digestion, nutrient homeostasis, and daily energy homeostasis 10. While starved flies show a cold preference, we show here that the food cue, such as the excitation of gustatory neurons, triggers a warm preference, and the nutritional value triggers an even higher warm preference. Thus, when flies exit the starvation state, they use a two-step approach to recovery, taste-evoked and nutrient-induced warm preferences. The taste-evoked warm preference in Drosophila may be a physiological response potentially equivalent to CPR in mammals. Furthermore, we found that internal needs, controlled by hunger signals and circadian clock genes, influence taste-evoked warm preference. Thus, we propose that the taste-evoked response plays an important role in recovery and represents another layer of regulation of energy homeostasis.

Tp is determined by the taste cue

Starved flies increase Tp in response to a nutrient-free taste cue (Fig. 1C and E), resulting in a taste-evoked warm preference. We showed that silencing of ACs or cold neurons caused a loss of taste-evoked warm preference (Figs. 3 and S3), and that excitation of ACs or cold neurons induced a taste-evoked warm preference (Fig. 3). The data suggest that both warm and cold neurons are important for taste-evoked warm preference: while ACs are required for the hunger-driven lower Tp 22, both ACs and cold neurons are likely to be important for this taste-evoked warm preference.

The hunger-driven lower Tp is a slower response because starvation gradually lowers their Tp, 22. In contrast, the taste-evoked warm preference is a rapid response. Once ACs and cold neurons are directly or indirectly activated, starved flies quickly move to a warmer area. This is interesting because even when ACs and cold neurons are activated by warm and cold, respectively, the activation of these neurons causes a warm preference. Given that the sensory detection of food (sight, smell, or chewing food) triggers CPR, the activation of these sensory neurons may induce CPR. Tp in sucralose-fed hungry flies is between that of fed and starved flies (Figs. 2, 3, and S3), making it difficult to detect the smaller temperature differences using the calcium imaging experiments. Therefore, we speculate that both ACs and cold neurons may facilitate rapid recovery from starvation so that flies can quickly return to their preferred temperature - body temperature - to a normal state.

Internal state influences taste-evoked warm preference

We show that mutants of genes involved in hunger and the circadian clock fail to show taste-evoked warm preference, suggesting that hunger and clock genes are important for taste-evoked warm preference. At a certain time of day, animals are hungry for food 44,65. Therefore, the "hungry" state acts as a gatekeeper, opening the gate of the circuits when hungry flies detect the food information that leads to taste-evoked warm preference (Fig. S4, blue arrow). While most hunger signals are important only for taste-evoked warm preference, some hunger signals are also required for both taste-evoked and nutrient-induced warm preferences (Fig. 4). The data suggest that internal state is more likely to be associated with taste-evoked warm preference than nutrient-induced warm preference. It is noteworthy that the sensory signals contribute to both taste-evoked warm preference and nutrient-induced warm preferences (Figs. S1, S3 and S4, blue and green arrows). Therefore, taste-evoked warm preference and nutrient-induced warm preference are differentiated at the level of the internal state, but not at the sensory level. This idea is analogous to appetitive memory formation. Sweet taste and nutrients regulate the different layers of the memory formation process. Recent evidence suggests that the rewarding process can be subdivided; the sweet taste is for short-term memory and the nutrient is for long-term memory 7577. The data suggest that the taste-evoked response functions differently from the nutrient-induced response. Therefore, taste sensation is not just the precursor to nutrient sensing/absorption, but plays an essential role in the rapid initiation of a taste-evoked behavior that would help the animal survive.

How do hunger signals or clock genes contribute to taste-evoked warm preference?

The hunger signaling hormones/peptides studied in this project are important for taste modulation. For example, mammalian NPY and its Drosophila homolog NPF modulate the output of taste signals 43,78,79. The AKH receptor is expressed in a subset of gustatory neurons that may modulate taste information for carbohydrate metabolism 80. Therefore, the hunger signals are likely to modulate the downstream of the sensory neurons, which may result in a taste-evoked warm preference.

Circadian clock genes control and coordinate the expression of many clock-controlled genes in the body 8184. Therefore, we expect that the absence of clock genes will disrupt the molecular and neural networks of cellular homeostasis, including metabolism, that are essential for animal life. For example, taste neurons express clock genes, and impaired clock function in taste neurons disrupts daily rhythms in feeding behavior 85. Temperature-sensing neurons transmit hot or cold temperature information to central clock neurons 8690. Therefore, the disrupted central clock in clock mutants may respond imprecisely to temperature signals. There are many possible reasons why the lack of clock gene expression in the brain is likely to cause abnormal taste-evoked warm preference.

In addition, hunger signals may contribute to the regulation of circadian output. DH44 is located in the dorsomedial region of the fly brain, the pars intercerebralis (PI), and DH44-expressing neurons play a role in the output pathway of the central clock 91,92. Insulin-producing cells (IPCs) are also located in addition to DH44-expressing neurons 9396. IPCs receive a variety of information, including circadian 92,97 and metabolic signals 6062,64. and then transduce the signals downstream to release Ilps. Both Upd2 and Ilp6, which are expressed in the fat body respond to metabolic states and remotely regulate Ilp expression 6062,64. Insect fat body is analogous to the fat tissues and liver in the vertebrates 98,99. Therefore, each hunger signal may have its specific function for taste-evoked warm preference. Further studies are needed to describe the entire process.

Taste-evoked warm preference may be CPR in flies

The introduction of food into the body disrupts the internal milieu, so CPR is a necessary process that helps animals prepare for digestion. Specifically, in mammals, taste leads to an immediate increase in body temperature and metabolic rate. Starvation results in lower body temperatures, and chewing food, even before it enters the stomach, triggers a rapid increase in heat production, demonstrating CPR in thermogenesis 13,14.

Starved flies have a lower Tp 22. Because Drosophila is a small ectotherm, the lower Tp indicates a lower body temperature 19,20. Even when the flies do not receive food, the sweet taste and the excitation of sweet neurons induce starved flies to show a warm preference, which eventually leads to a warmer body temperature. Thus, the taste-evoked warm preference in Drosophila may be a physiological response equivalent to one of the CPRs observed in mammals. Thus, in both flies and mammals, starvation leads to lower body temperatures, and food cues quickly initiate a rise in body temperature before food enters their body.

CPR is essential because both starved mammals and starved flies must rapidly regulate their body temperature to survive

As soon as starving flies taste food, the sensory signals trigger CPR. They can move to a warmer place to prepare to raise their body temperature (Fig. S4, blue arrows). CPR may allow flies to choose a more hospitable place to restore their physiological state and allow for higher metabolism, and eventually they move to the next step, such as foraging and actively searching for a mate before competitors arrive. As such, CPR may be a strategy for the fly’s survival. Similarly, starvation or malnutrition in mammals causes lower body temperatures 6,7,100, and biting food triggers heat production, which is CPR 1315. Thus, while starvation causes lower body temperatures in both flies and mammals, food cues initiate CPR in body temperature elevation and nutrient intake, resulting in full recovery from the starved state. Our data suggest that Drosophila CPR may be a physiological response equivalent to CPR observed in other animals. Thus, Drosophila may shed new light on the regulation of CPR and provide a deeper understanding of the relationship between CPR and metabolism.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to Drs. Anupama Dahanukar, Hubert Amrein, Greg Suh, and Paul A. Garrity and the Bloomington Drosophila Fly Stock Center for the fly lines. We thank members of the Hamada laboratory for critical comments and advice on the manuscript, Dr. Richard A. Lang and members of his laboratory for comments and kindly sharing reagents, Dr. Satoshi Namekawa for kindly sharing reagents, and Matthew Batie and Nathan T. Petts for design and construction of the behavioral assays and red-light illumination apparatus. This research was supported by a Trustee Grant and RIP funding from Cincinnati Children’s Hospital, JST (Japan Science and Technology)/Precursory Research for Embryonic Science and Technology (PRESTO), the March of Dimes, and NIH R01 grant GM107582 and NIH R21 grant NS112890 to F.N.H. S.H.S. is a Latin American Fellow in the Biomedical Sciences supported by the Pew Charitable Trusts. Research in the laboratory of J.C.C. is supported by NIH R01 grant DK124068.

Contributions

F.N.H and Y.U designed the experiments. Y.U, E.N., T.N. and J.S. performed the temperature preference behavioral assays and data analysis. Y.U., S.H.S., S.S.U. and J.C. performed the feeding experiments and the data analysis. F.N.H and Y.U wrote the manuscript.

Declaration of Interest

The authors declare no competing interests□

The starvation period is unlikely to affect the ability to recover (Related to Fig. 3)

Comparison of Tp of w1118 control flies between fed (white bar), starved (STV; gray bar), and refed (STV; gray bar) and refed (blue or green bar). Flies were starved for overnight (STV1ON; gray bar) or 1.5 ON (STV1.5ON; dark gray bar), i.e. starved for 18-21 hrs. starved for 18-21 hrs or 26-29 hrs. Starved flies were refed with sucralose for 10 min (blue bar) or glucose for 10 min (green bar). These behavioral experiments were performed on ZT4-7. Dots on each bar indicate individual Tp in the assays. Numbers in italics indicate the number of trials. Per Shapiro-Wilk test was performed to test for normality. One-way ANOVA or Kruskal-Wallis test was used for statistical analysis. Green stars indicate Tukey’s post hoc test or Dunn’s test was used to compare between each experiment and starved condition. and starved condition. All data shown are means with sem. *p < 0.05. **p < 0.01. ***p < 0.001. ****p < 0.0001. NS indicates no significance. The same data from STV1ON, STV1ON+refed sucralose for 10 min and STV1ON+refed sucralose for 10 min in Fig. 1C-E are used in Fig. S1.

Number of touches to water, sucralose and glucose using yw, per01 and tim01 flies. (Related to Figures 1, 5, and 6)

Feeding assay: The number of touches to water (gray), 2.8 mM (equivalent to 5%) glucose (green), or 2.8 mM sucralose solution (blue) was examined using yw, per01, or tim01 flies starved for 1 day. Cumulative number of touches for 0-30 min was plotted. Two-way ANOVA was used for multiple comparisons. Blue and green stars show Fisher’s LSD post hoc test comparing sucralose (blue stars) or glucose (green stars) solution feeding to water drinking. Black stars show the statistical difference between sucralose and glucose refeeding. All data shown are means with SEM. *p < 0.05. *F*p < 0.01. ***p < 0.001. ****p < 0.0001.

Taste- or temperature-sensing neurons are not important for nutrient- (carbohydrate-) induced warm preference (Related to Figures 2 and 3).

Comparison of Tp of flies between fed (white bar), starved (STV; gray bar), and refed (orange, blue, green, or dark green bar) conditions. Starved flies were refed with fly food for 10 min (fly food; orange bar), sucralose for 10 min (blue bar), or glucose for 10 min (green bar) or 1 hr (dark green bar). These behavioral experiments were conducted on ZT4-7. The dots on each bar indicate individual Tp in the assays. Numbers in italics indicate the number of experiments. The Shapiro-Wilk test was performed to test for normality. One-way ANOVA or Kruskal-Wallis test was used for statistical analysis. Green stars indicate Tukey’s post hoc test or Dunn’s test compared between each experiment and the starved condition. All data shown are means with sem. *p < 0.05. **p < 0.01. ***p < 0.001. ****p < 0.0001. NS indicates no significance. The same data from UAS-Kir/+ in Fig. 2E are used in Fig. S3I.

Model

A schematic diagram of Tp recovery by taste-evoked warm preference (blue arrows) and nutrient-induced warm preference (green arrows).

Materials and Methods

All flies were reared under 12 hr-light/12 hr-dark (LD) cycles at 25°C and 60-70% humidity in an incubator (DRoS33SD, Powers Scientific Inc.) with an electric timer (light on: 8am; light off: 8pm). The light intensity is 1000-1400 lux. All flies were reared on custom fly food recipe, with the following composition per 1 L of food: 6.0 g sucrose, 7.3 g agar, 44.6 g cornmeal, 22.3 g yeast and 16.3 mL molasses as described previously 52. white1118 (w1118) and yellow1 white1 (y1w1) flies were used as control flies. EP5Δ; Gr64a1 (Gr5a-/-; Gr64a-/-) was kindly provided by Dr. Anupama Dahanukar 41. R1; Gr5a-LexA; +; ΔGr61a, ΔGr64a-f (Gr5a-/-; Gr61a-/-, Gr64a-f-/-) were kindly provided by Dr. Hubert Amrein 45,46. Dh44-Gal4 was kindly provided by Dr. Greg Suh 42. TrpA1SH-gal4 was kindly provided by Dr. Paul A. Garrity 38. Other fly lines were provided by Bloomington Drosophila stock center and Vienna Drosophila Stock Center.

Temperature preference behavioral assay

The temperature preference behavior assays were examined using a gradient of temperatures, ranging from 16-34°C and were performed for 30 min in an environmental room maintained at 25°C /60%–70% humidity as described previously 22. We prepared a total of 40-50 flies (male and female flies were mixed) for fed condition experiments and 90-100 flies for overnight(s) starved and refed condition experiments for one trial. Flies were never reused in subsequent trials. In starved and refed experiments, we prepared double the amount of flies required for one trial, as almost half of them died by starvation stress. Others climbed on the wall and ceiling (starved flies are usually hyperactive (Yang et al. 2015 PNAS)), even though we applied slippery coating chemicals (PTFE; Cat# 665800, Sigma or byFormica PTFE Plus, https://byformica.com/products/fluon-plus-ptfe-escape-prevention-coating) to the plexiglass covers.

The behavioral assays were performed for 30 min at Zeitgeber time (ZT) 4-7 (Light on and light off are defined as ZT0 and ZT12, respectively), and starvation was initiated at ZT9-10 (starved for 1, 2 or 3 O/N are 18-21 hrs, 42-45 hrs or 66-69 hrs, respectively). As for STV1.5 for Fig. S1, starvation was initiated at ZT1-2 (26-29 hrs). For the starvation assays, the collected flies were maintained with our fly food for at least one day for their maturation and subsequently moved into plastic vials with 3 mL of distilled water which is absorbed by a Kimwipe paper. For the refeeding experiments, starved flies were transferred into plastic vials with 2 mL of 2.8 mM sugar solution (Sucralose water/ Glucose water) for a certain time period. Sugar solution is absorbed by the half size of a Kimwipe.

After the 30-min behavioral assay, the number of flies whose bodies were completely located on the apparatus were counted. Flies whose bodies were partially or completely located on the walls of the apparatus cover were not included in the data analysis. The percentage of flies within each one-degree temperature interval on the apparatus was calculated by dividing the number of flies within each one-degree interval by the total number of flies on the apparatus. The location of each one-degree interval was determined by measuring the temperature at 6 different points on the bottom of the apparatus. Data points were plotted as the percentage of flies within a one-degree temperature interval. The weighted mean of Tp was calculated by totaling the product of the percentage of flies within a one-degree temperature interval and the corresponding temperature (e.g., fractional number of flies x 15.5°C + fractional number of flies x 16.5°C +……… fractional number of flies x 34.5°C). If the SEM of the averaged Tp was not < 0.3 after the five trials, we performed additional trials around 10 times until the SEM reached < 0.3.

Temperature preference rhythm (TPR) assay

For the TPR assays, we performed temperature preference behavior assays in different time windows during the day (zeitgeber time (ZT) or circadian time (CT) 1-3, 4-6, 7-9, and 10-12) as described previously 66,101. Because starvation duration directly affects flies’ Tp 22, starvation was initiated at each time window to adjust the starvation duration at each time point, which means flies were starved for 24 hrs or 48 hrs but not 1 or 2 O/N. Each behavioral assay was not examined during these time periods (ZT or CT0-1 and 11.5-13) because of large phenotype variation around light on and light off.

Optogenetic activation

For the optogenetic activation of the target neurons for behavioral assays, red-light-sensitive channelrodopsin, UAS-CsChrimson, was crossed with each Gal4 drivers. Flies were reared on fly food at 25°C and 60-70% humidity under LD cycles in an incubator (DRoS33SD, Powers Scientific Inc.) with an electric timer. After flies emerged, we collected adult flies and kept them with fly food for 1-2 days. Next day, flies were transferred into water with or without 0.8 mM all-trans retinal (ATR; #R2500, Sigma) diluted in dimethyl sulfoxide (DMSO; #472301, Sigma) for 2 O/N. To activate flies expressing UAS-CsChrimson crossed with Gal4 drivers, we employed 627 nm red-colored light-emitted diode (LED) equipped with pulse photoillumination system (10 Hz, 0.08 mWmm-2). Flies were irradiated with pulsed red light for 10 min, which is equivalent to refeeding time period. This photoillumination system was used in an incubator (Sanyo Scientific, MIR-154), and followed by temperature preference behavioral assays.

Optogenetic assay for tubGal80ts; R11F02-Gal4>uas-CsChrimson

Because R11F02-Gal4>uas-CsChrimson flies died during the pupal stage, Gal4/Gal80ts system was used to restrict uas-CsChrimson expression to the adult stage for optogenetc assays against the cold-sensing neurons (R11F02-Gal4-expressing neurons). Gal80ts is a temperature sensitive allele of Gal80, which binds Gal4 and inhibits CsChrimson gene expression at 18°C. However, Gal80 is degraded at 29°C; in turn, the Gal4 activates CsChrimson gene expression (Southall et al. 2008 CSH protocol). tubGal80ts; R11F02-Gal4>uas-CsChrimson flies were reared with fly food at 18°C. Emerged adult flies were collected and kept with fly food at 29°C. Next day, flies were transferred to water condition (starved condition) with or without 0.8 mM ATR for 2 O/N. Starved flies with or without ATR application were exposed to pulsed red light for 10 min (equivalent to refeeding period) and then immediately loaded in the behavior apparatus for behavioral assays to measure their Tp.

Feeding assay

To measure single fly feeding, we used Drosophila Fly Liquid Interaction Counter (FLIC) system (Ro et al. 2013 Plos one; Sable systems International https://www.sablesys.com). Groups of 1-2-day-old male and female flies were starved for 24 hrs, which was started from between ZT4-5 (12-1pm). Individual flies were then loaded into FLIC monitors. Flies were acclimatized to the monitors for 30 min, with access to water provided in the food wells. To start the feeding assay, water was replaced with 2.8 mM Sucralose or 2.8 mM Glucose solution (equivalent to 5% Glucose conc.) and the number of licks (touches) were recorded for 30 min. Assays were performed at ZT4-7. To account for potential confounding effect of startle response when changing the food, wells where water was replaced with new water were used as a control. FLIC raw data was analyzed using the FLIC R code master (Pletcher Lab, available at https://github.com/PletcherLab/FLIC_R_Code). The numbers of licks were obtained and summed into 5-min windows bin, while the cumulative licks were obtained by the consecutive sum of the licks in these bins.